Did you arrive here by via search engine?
Click here to view the original version of this article

Click to Print This Page
(This section will not print)

The Weathering Hypothesis of Aging: Part I

Course Authors

Bruce S. McEwen, Ph.D.

Dr. McEwen reports no commercial conflict of interest.

Estimated course time: 1 hour(s).

Albert Einstein College of Medicine – Montefiore Medical Center designates this enduring material activity for a maximum of 1.0 AMA PRA Category 1 Credit(s)™. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity.

In support of improving patient care, this activity has been planned and implemented by Albert Einstein College of Medicine-Montefiore Medical Center and InterMDnet. Albert Einstein College of Medicine – Montefiore Medical Center is jointly accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education (ACCME), the Accreditation Council for Pharmacy Education (ACPE), and the American Nurses Credentialing Center (ANCC), to provide continuing education for the healthcare team.

 
Learning Objectives

Upon completion of this Cyberounds®, you should be able to:

  • Discuss the structure and function of the hippocampal region of the brain

  • Describe how aging of the brain, especially the hippocampus, is assessed

  • Discuss the evidence for and against neuronal loss in the aging brain.

 

Introduction

We are acutely aware of the aging process in ourselves and in our friends and loved ones and cognizant of the considerable individual differences in the rates of aging. This is particularly true for those aspects of aging which impair mental and physical functioning. How much of this is in our genes and how much is environmentally driven? There is no simple answer but it is important to understand how our experiences can influence the rate at which we age, i.e., the "weathering" of our bodies and our brains.

A recent article in the New England Journal of Medicine provided data that a lifetime of experience of economic hardship resulted in earlier declines in physical functioning, cognitive performance and mental health.(1) The concept of allostatic load,(2) which was developed in four previous Cyberounds® conferences, provides the basis for understanding how stress hormones can produce wear and tear on the body and brain.

It is the brain that is particularly important for the aging process because it controls neuroendocrine and neural processes that affect metabolism, cardiovascular and immune function. Yet, the nervous system lacks the ability to generate new nerve cells. Once enough neurons die there is a permanent loss of function. However, there is controversy concerning how many neurons die in the aging brain. There are, moreover, alternative explanations for loss of function in the aging brain that are not so drastic and are potentially treatable.

This Cyberounds® is the first of four that addresses some of the current information and controversies regarding the "weathering" of the brain. The series focuses on a region of the brain, the hippocampus, which is particularly sensitive to the aging process, in part because it is very sensitive to the effects of stress. This Cyberounds® considers how we assess aging and whether the changes seen in animals and humans in cognitive function are necessarily due to permanent loss of nerve cells.

Importance of the Hippocampus

The hippocampus is a particularly vulnerable and sensitive region of the brain that is also very important for declarative and spatial learning and memory.(3) Input to the hippocampus comes from the entorhinal cortex and this input is the first to degenerate in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease.(4) The cross-sectional 'tri-synaptic' organization of the hippocampus is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Tri-Synaptic Organization of the Hippocampus.

Figure 1

The first synapses are those from the entorhinal cortex input to the dentate gyrus; the second consist of the dentate gyrus projection to the CA3 neurons; while the third synapses are those from the CA3 to the CA1 neurons. Through this tri-synaptic circuit flows information from the cortex which is processed and distributed to other brain regions, including the subcortical regions of the brain.(5)

Hippocampal neurons are like high-performance race cars -- they have enormous capabilities in terms of their activity and plasticity but, as a result of these characteristics, are particularly vulnerable to seizures, strokes and head trauma, as well as stressful experiences.(6),(8) Their remarkable plasticity is demonstrated by long-term synaptic potentiation and depression, dendritic remodelling, synaptic turnover and neurogenesis in the case of the dentate gyrus.(7),(9)

The work of aus der Muhlen and Ockenfels(10) first drew attention to potentially toxic actions of adrenal steroids. These investigators reported darkly stained neurons in the hippocampus of guinea pigs exposed to high levels of glucocorticoids, an observation that has been confirmed and extended to repeated stress in subsequent studies,(11),(12) although there are still some doubts as to whether 'dark neurons' may be artifacts of tissue trauma.(13) In 1968, we discovered receptors for adrenal steroids in the hippocampus(14) and it is now known that two types of adrenal steroid receptors exist in the hippocampus and other brain regions and mediate a variety of adrenal steroid effects on excitability, neurochemistry and structure.(8)

Phillip Landfield(15) and then Robert Sapolsky(16),(17) provided evidence for a role of adrenal steroids in neuronal aging in the hippocampus, leading to the formulation of the "glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis."(6) This hypothesis states that glucocorticoids participate in a feed-forward cascade of effects on the brain and body in which progressive glucocorticoid-induced damage to the hippocampus promotes progressive elevation of adrenal steroids and dysregulation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis.(6) According to this view, "weathering" is a process that can accelerate with increasing dysregulation of the HPA axis and provides a gradually increasing allostatic load.

Subsequent work has provided considerable support for the model for both brain and body aging and has extended our knowledge of the aging hippocampus from animal models to humans.(18),(19),(20) At the same time, the new information has revealed a number of complications and problems with the original formulation.

First, finding neuronal death in the hippocampus during aging is technically very problematic. Recent information from stereological cell counting has down-played the importance of neuronal death, as opposed to declining neuronal function and various forms of structural plasticity. This will be discussed in this Cyberounds®.

Second, the role of the hippocampus in HPA regulation is more complex than originally believed, considering both its neuroanatomical connections to the hypothalamus and the nature of negative feedback regulation of the HPA axis. Moreover, there is a life-long pattern of reactivity of the HPA axis which helps to set the rate of 'weathering" of the brain and body. This will be discussed in Part II of this Cyberounds® series.

Third, the hippocampus is a dynamic and plastic region of the adult as well as developing brain, in which stress hormones (glucocorticoids) have an important role; yet, glucocorticoids do not act alone on the hippocampus and there is new information down-playing the relative importance of adrenal steroids, as opposed to excitatory amino acids and other modulators, including neurotrophins and calcium ions, as well as sex hormones. This will be the subject of the third Cyberounds® in this series.

Fourth, our new appreciation of the plasticity of the hippocampus has opened the way to possible treatment strategies to reverse hippocampal atrophy and retard the onset and progression of Alzheimer's disease. This will be discussed in the fourth Cyberounds®.

Obtaining an Accurate Description of the Aging Brain and Body

Chronological age alone is not sufficient to predict the state of the aging brain and body. The problem that individual differences poses for aging research is that individual animals or human subjects must be evaluated for cognitive and physical status, relative to other individuals of the same age, in order to understand the meaning of any single physiological, neuroanatomical, neurochemical or molecular measure.(21) For the hippocampus, as well as for the rest of the body, additional information is needed regarding both cognitive function and stress mediators (i.e., HPA and sympathetic nervous system reactivity), as well as measures of body function that are influenced by these mediators. This is true not only for animal models(22) but also for studies on humans, for which longitudinal information about HPA activity and other measures of allostatic load(2) in aging subjects have been shown to predict cognitive decline and onset of cardiovascular disease.(23),(24),(25) Thus, the most meaningful studies have been done comparing cognitively-impaired and -unimpaired groups of aging individuals and in groups of individuals differing in their levels of allostatic load.(23),(24),(25) We shall return to the topic of individual differences in Part II.

Neuron Death Is Not Inevitable or the Only Cause of Age-related Impairment

There has been a tendency to interpret the "glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis" as implying that hippocampal neuron death is an inevitable consequence of brain aging and that age-related impairments in cognitive function are solely related to such neuronal loss. This notion has given away to a more flexible view of brain aging, in which impairments in hippocampal functioning can be studied in terms of potentially reversible, as well as irreversible, changes in neuronal structure, neurochemistry and function.(26),(27)

The first challenge to the notion of neuronal loss is from a methodological problem in estimating neuron number in the brain. Because of the gradual time course of brain aging, even in rapidly-aging small animals like rats, observing neuronal death by counting dying neurons is futile because it would have to be such a slow process to account for the gradual changes in function. For that reason, studies of pyramidal neuron damage and the role and mechanism of action of glucocorticoids and excitatory amino acids have utilized kainic acid damage or transient ischemia models. In the aging brain, counting neurons to determine the average decline in neuronal content of the hippocampus is also fraught with technical problems. The initial reports of the aging hippocampus identified a reduced density of pyramidal neurons in aging rats that also showed impairments of spatial and other memory tasks.(29) However, a neuroanatomist by the name of Mark West introduced a stereological counting procedure(30) and reported a failure to find reduced hippocampal neuron number in aging rats with memory impairment compared to aging rats without impairment.(31) A similar negative finding was recently published in another study of aging cognitively impaired versus unimpaired rats.(32)

Even while the issue of age-related neuronal loss is controversial, and by no means resolved, there is evidence that the aging hippocampus undergoes progressive changes with age in calcium homeostasis, the plasticity of response to glucocorticoids and in the expression of markers related to neuroprotection and damage. The activity of L-type calcium channels undergoes an increase in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons of aging rats and results in an increased after-hyperpolarization(28) which alters the electrophysiological properties of these neurons and makes them more vulnerable to damage. In cultured embryonic hippocampal neurons that are maintained for 28d, there is an increase in calcium channel activity and in after-hyperpolarization that is accompanied by decreased neuronal survival; blocking L-type calcium channels increased neuronal survival.(33) It is interesting to note that the increased after-hyperpolarization is associated with an enhanced induction of long-term depression (LTD) in CA1 pyramidal neurons and an impaired induction of long-term potentiation (LTP).(34) Thus, insofar as LTP and LTD may be related to synaptic plasticity during learning,(35) these age-related changes suggest a possible basis for cognitive impairment in aging rats.(34)

Glucocorticoids enhance calcium channel activity and after-hyperpolarization(36) and glucocorticoid receptor expression shows a progressive failure of negative feedback regulation in old versus young rats. In young rats, repeated stress causes a down-regulation of glucocorticoid receptor levels, thus decreasing glucocorticoid efficacy on various target genes, whereas, with increasing age, this down-regulation is lost, thus preserving glucocorticoid actions.(29) This natural mechanism in the young hippocampus (repeated stress ---> reduced magnitude of the glucocorticoid feedback signal ---> reduced impact of glucocorticoids on calcium channel activity, among other effects) may be protective, for increased calcium channel activity contributes to free radical generation and other processes that may damage neurons.(37),(38) With the loss of stress-induced down-regulation of glucocorticoid receptors, older rats appear to lose this protective device and may be more vulnerable to increased levels of glucocorticoids which do appear to accompany aging, particularly in cognitively-impaired rats.(29)

Even if outright neuronal loss is not a major event in the aging hippocampus of cognitively-impaired rats, there are indications that gene products associated with neurodegeneration and damage are differentially regulated in the aging-impaired brain, compared to unimpaired aging rats and young rats, although the interpretation of the results is very complex.(39) In aging, cognitively-impaired rats, the levels of mRNA for the 695 amino acid form of the beta amyloid precursor protein (betaAPP) and for the magnesium-dependent superoxide dismutase (Mg-SOD) were both elevated throughout the hippocampus compared with young rats at the same time the levels of the betaAPP protein and Mg-SOD protein were both depressed. Levels of mRNA for glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), a marker of astrocytes, which increases with damage, were elevated in the hippocampus of aging, cognitively impaired rats, although the level of the GFAP protein was not elevated. Since betaAPP gives rise to both a toxic beta amyloid protein and a protective secreted form, the reduced levels of betaAPP expression in aging, cognitively impaired rats is difficult to interpret without a separate measurement of the two forms of the protein. On the other hand, lower Mg-SOD protein is consistent with a lower capacity for free-radical scavenging and an increased risk for free-radical induced neural damage.(40)

Another aspect of the aging hippocampus is alteration in glutamate release associated with an age-related increase in dynorphin content of the hippocampus.(41) Dynorphin is a peptide that was originally identified as one of the "endogenous opioids" and which now is known to function as a neurotransmitter in some nerve cells. Dynorphin appears to be co-present in mossy fiber nerve terminals with glutamate, and is release by these terminals during nerve stimulation. The age-related increase in dynorphin content changes is accompanied by impairments of spatial learning, something that is increasingly evident in some rats more than others as they get older. Since dynorphin is present in the mossy fiber pathway as a co-transmitter with glutamate, the increased levels of this peptide may have an inhibitory autoregulatory function at the mossy fiber synapse,(41) resulting in less glutamate release and impairing the synaptic transmission and plasticity that is essential for learning and memory.

Conclusions

What a few years ago was thought to be an open-and-shut case of age- related and stress-induced neuronal loss has turned out to be far more complicated and interesting as far as the multiple processes that appear to be involved in the aging of the hippocampus. Indeed, neuronal loss with aging may have been over-emphasized, so much so that the irreversibility of the age-related changes is called into question. There are a number of aspects of these new findings that will be addressed in the next three Cyberounds®. The first issue is the role of the HPA axis and glucocorticoids as the basis for the "weathering" of the hippocampus and as the reason for individual differences in the rate of "weathering," a subject we will discuss in Part II of this series.

Acknowledgments

Research in the author's laboratory on some of the topics discussed in this article is supported by NIH Grants NS07080 and MH41256 and by the Health Foundation (New York), Servier (France) and UCB (Belgium).


Footnotes

1Lynch, J. W., G. A. Kaplan, and S. J. Shema. 1997. Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. NEJM 337:1889-1895.
2McEwen, B. S. 1998. Protective and Damaging Effects of Stress Mediators. New England J. Med. 338:171-179.
3Eichenbaum, H. 1997. How does the brain organize memories? Science 277:330-332.
4Braak, H. and E. Braak. 1997. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol. Aging 18:351-357.
5Risold, P. Y. and L. W. Swanson. 1997. Structural evidence for functional domains in the rat hippocampus. Science 272:1484-1486.
6Sapolsky, R. M., L. C. Krey, and B. S. McEwen. 1986. The Neuroendocrinology of Stress and Aging: The Glucocorticoid Cascade Hypothesis. Endocr. Rev. 7:284-301.
7McEwen, B. S., D. Albeck, H. Cameron, H. M. Chao, E. Gould, N. Hastings, Y. Kuroda, V. Luine, A. M. Magarinos, C. R. McKittrick, M. Orchinik, C. Pavlides, P. Vaher, Y. Watanabe, and N. Weiland. 1995. Stress and the Brain: A Paradoxical Role for Adrenal Steroids. In Vitamins And Hormones. G. D. Litwack, editor. Academic Press, Inc. 371-402.
8DeKloet, E. R., E. Vreugdenhil, M. S. Oitzl, and M. Joels. 1998. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocrine Rev. 19:269-301.
9Cameron, H. A. and E. Gould. 1996. The Control of Neuronal Birth and Survival. In Receptor Dynamics in Neural Development. C. A. Shaw, editor. CRC Press, New York. 141-157.
10aus der Muhlen, K. and H. Ockenfels. 1969. Morphologische veranderungen im diencephalon und telencephalon: storungen des regelkreises adenohypophysenebennierenrinde. Z. Zellforsch. Mikrosck. Anat. 93:126-141.
11Mizoguchi, K., T. Kunishita, D. H. Chui, and T. Tabira. 1992. Stress induces neuronal death in the hippocampus of castrated rats. Neurosci. Letts. 138:157-160.
12Fuchs, E., H. Uno, and G. Flugge. 1995. Chronic psychosocial stress induces morphological alterations in hippocampal pyramidal neurons of the tree shrew. Brain Res 673:275-282.
13Cammermeyer, J. 1978. Is the solitary dark neuron a manifestation of postmortem trauma to the brain inadequately fixed by perfusion? Histochemistry 56:97-115.
14McEwen, B. S., J. Weiss, and L. Schwartz. 1968. Selective retention of corticosterone by limbic structures in rat brain. Nature 220:911-912.
15Landfield, P., J. Waymire, and G. Lynch. 1978. Hippocampal aging and adrenocorticoids: quantitative correlation. Science 202:1098-1101.
16Sapolsky, R., L. Krey, and B. S. McEwen. 1984. Glucocorticoid-sensitive hippocampal neurons are involved in terminating the adrenocortical stress response. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.USA 81:6174-6177.
17Sapolsky, R., L. Krey, and B. S. McEwen. 1985. Prolonged glucocorticoid exposure reduces hippocampal neuron number: implications for aging. J. Neurosci. 5:1222-1227.
18Sapolsky, R. 1992. Stress, the Aging Brain and the Mechanisms of Neuron Death. Cambridge MIT Press 1-423.
19McEwen, B. S. 1997. Possible mechanisms for atrophy of the human hippocampus. Molecular Psychiatry 2:255-262.
20Sapolsky, R. M. 1996. Why Stress Is Bad for Your Brain. Science 273:749-750.
21McEwen, B. S. 1992. Re-examination of the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis of stress and aging. In Progress in Brain Research. D. Swaab, M. Hoffman, R. Mirmiran, F. Ravid, and F. van Leeuwen, editors. Elsevier, Amsterdam. 365-383.
22Meaney, M., D. Aitken, H. Berkel, S. Bhatnager, and R. Sapolsky. 1988. Effect of neonatal handlng of age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science 239:766-768.
23Lupien, S., A. R. Lecours, I. Lussier, G. Schwartz, N. P. V. Nair, and M. J. Meaney. 1994. Basal cortisol levels and cognitive deficits in human aging. J. Neurosci. 14:2893-2903.
24Seeman, T. E., B. S. McEwen, B. H. Singer, M. S. Albert, and J. W. Rowe. 1997. Increase in Urinary Cortisol Excretion and Memory Declines: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 82:2458-2465.
25Lupien, S. J., M. J. DeLeon, S. De Santi, A. Convit, C. Tarshish, N. P. V. Nair, M. Thakur, B. S. McEwen, R. L. Hauger, and M. J. Meaney. 1998. Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nature Neuroscience 1:69-73.
26Morrison, J. H. and P. R. Hof. 1997. Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science 278:412-419.
27Landfield, P. W. 1987. Modulation of brain aging correlates by long-term alterations of adrenal steroids and neurally-active peptides. Progress in Brain Research 72:279-300.
28Landfield, P. W. and J. C. Eldridge. 1994. Evolving aspects of the glucocorticoid hypothesis of brain aging: Hormonal modulation of neuronal calcium homeostasis. Neurobiol. Aging 15:579-588.
29Kerr, S., L. Campbell, M. Applegate, A. Brodish, and P. Landfield. 1991. Chronic stress-induced acceleration of electrophysiologic and morphometric biomarkers of hippocampal aging. J. Neurosci. 11:1316-1324.
30Lindvall-Axelsson, M., P. Hedner, and C. Owman. 1989. Corticosteroid action on choroid plexus: reduction in Na+-K+-ATPase activity, choline transport capacity, and rate of CSF formation. Exp. Brain Res. 77:605-610.
31Rasmussen, T., T. Schliemann, J. C. Sorensen, J. Zimmer, and M. J. West. 1996. Memory Impaired Aged Rats: No loss of Principal Hippocampal and Subicular Neurons. Neurobiol. Aging 14:143-147.
32Rapp, P. R. and M. Gallagher. 1996. Preserved neuron number in the hippocampus of aged rats with spatial learning deficits. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.USA 93:9926-9930.
33Porter, N. M., O. Thibault, V. Thibault, K. Chen, and P. W. Landfield. 1997. Calcium channel density and hippocampal cell death with age in long-term culture. J. Neurosci. 17:5629-5639.
34Norris, C. M., S. Halpain, and T. C. Foster. 1998. Reversal of age-related alterations in synaptic plasticity by blockade of L-type Ca2+ channels. J Neurosci 18:3171-3179.
35Maren, S. 1995. Properties and mechanisms of long-term synaptic plasticity in the mammalian brain: relationships to learning and memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 63:1-18.
36Joels, M. 1997. Steroid hormones and excitability in the mammalian brain. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 18:2-48.
37McCord, J. 1985. Oxygen-derived free radicals in postischemic tissue injury. New Engl. J. Med. 312:159-163.
38Liu, J., X. Wang, M. K. Shigenaga, H. C. Yeo, A. Mori, and B. N. Ames. 1996. Immobilization stress causes oxidative damage ot lipid, protein and DNA in the brain or rats. FASEB J. 10:1532-1538.
39Sugaya, K., M. Chouinard, R. Greene, M. Robbins, D. Personett, C. Kent, M. Gallagher, and M. McKinney. 1996. Molecular Indices of Neuronal and Glial Plasticity in the Hippocmapl Formation in a Rodent Model of Age-Induced Spatial Learning Impairment. The Journal of Neuroscience 16:3427-3443.
40Chan, P. 1996. Role of oxidants in ischemic brain damage. Stroke 27:1124-1129.
41Zhang, W., W. R. Mundy, L. Thai, P. M. Hudson, M. Gallagher, H. A. Tilson, and J. S. Hong. 1991. Decreased glutamate release correlates with elevated dynorphin content in the hippocampus of aged rats with spatial learning deficits. Hippocampus 1:391-398.